Understanding Understanding
“The greatest enemy of knowledge is not ignorance; it is
the illusion of knowledge.” – Steven Hawking
Many
researchers in the field of psychology don’t appear to understand that
restating a finding is not the same as explaining that finding. For
instance, if you found that men are more likely to gamble than women, a
typical form of “explanation” of this finding would be to say that men
have more of a “risk bias” than women, resulting in them gambling more.
Clearly this explanation doesn’t add anything that stating the finding
didn’t; all it manages to do is add a label to the finding. Now some
psychologists might understand this shortcoming and take the next step:
they might say something along the lines of men perceive gambling to be
more fun or more likely to payoff than women do. While that might well
be true, it still falls short of an complete explanation. Instead, it
would merely push the explanation stage back a step to a question about
why men might perceive gambling differently than women do. If the
researchers understand this further shortcoming and take the next step,
they’ll reference some cause of that feeling. If we’re lucky, that cause
will be non-circular and amount to more than the phrase “culture did
it”.
The smart money
is on betting against that outcome, though…
A good explanation needs to focus on some outcome of a
behavior; some plausible function of that outcome that can account for
the emotion or feeling itself. This is notably easier in some cases than
others: hunger
motivates people to seek out and consume food avoiding starvation; fear motivates people to
escape from or avoid threatening situations, avoiding danger; guilt
motivates people to make amends and repair relationships towards wronged
parties, avoiding condemnation and punishment
while reaping the benefits of social interaction. Recently, I found
myself posing that functional question about a feeling that is not often
discussed: understanding.
Teasing out the function of understanding is by no means a
straightforward task. Before undertaking the task, however, I need to
make a key distinction concerning precisely what I mean by
“understanding”. After all,
if wikipedia has a
hard time defining the term, I can’t just
assume that we’ll all be the on the same page despite using the same
word.The distinction I would like to draw is between understanding
per se and the feeling of understanding. The examples given on wikipedia
reflect understanding per se: the ability to draw connections among
mental representations. Understanding per se, then, represents the
application of knowledge. If a rat has learned to press a bar for food,
for instance, we would say that the rat understands something about the
connection between bar pressing and receiving food, in that the former
seems to cause the latter. The degree of understanding per se can vary
in terms of accuracy and completeness. To continue on with the rat
example, a rat can understand that pressing the bar generally leads to
it receiving food without understanding the mechanisms through which the
process works. Similarly, a person might understand that taking an
allergy pill will result in their allergy symptoms being reduced, but
their understanding of how that process works might be substantially
less detailed or accurate than the understanding of the researchers
responsible for developing the pill.
Understanding per se is to be
distinguished from the feeling of understanding. While understanding
per se refers to the actual connections among your mental
representations, the feeling of understanding refers to your mental
representations about the state of those other mental representations.
The feeling of understanding, then, is a bit of a metacognitive
sensation; your thinking about your thinking. Much like understanding
per se, the feeling of understanding comes in varying degrees: one can
feel as if they don’t understand something at all through feeling as if
they understand it completely, and anything in between. With this
distinction made, we can begin to start considering some profitable
questions: what is the connection between understanding per se and the
feeling of understanding? What behaviors are encouraged by the feeling
of understanding? What functional outcome(s) are those behaviors aimed
at achieving? Given these functional outcomes, what predictions can we
draw about how people experiencing various degrees of feeling as if they
understand something will react to certain contexts?
Maybe even what
Will Smith meant when he wrote “Parents Just Don’t Understand“
To begin to answer these questions, let’s return to the
initial quote. The enemy of knowledge is not ignorance, but rather the
illusion of knowledge; the feeling of understanding. While a bit on the
dramatic and poetic sides of things, the quote brings to light an
important idea: there is not necessarily a perfect correlation between
understanding per se and the feeling of understanding. Sure,
understanding per se might tend to trigger feelings of understanding,
but we ought to be concerned with matters of degree. It is clear that
increased feelings of understanding do not require a tight connection to
degrees of understanding per se. In much the same way, one’s judgment
of how attractive they are need not perfectly correlate with how
attractive they actually are. This is a partial, if relatively
underspecified, answer to our first question. Thankfully, it is all my
account of understanding requires: a less than perfect correlation
between understanding per se and feelings of understanding.This
brings us to the second question: what behaviors are motivated by the
feeling of understanding. If you’re a particularly astute reader, you’ll
have noticed that the term “understanding” appeared several times in
the first paragraph. In each instance, it referred to researchers
feeling that their understanding per se was incomplete. What did this
feeling motivate researchers to do? Continue to attempt and build their
understanding per se. In the cases where researchers lack the feeling
that their understanding per se was incomplete, they seem to do one
thing: stop. That is to say that reaching a feeling of understanding
appears to act as a stopping rule for learning. That people stop
investing in learning when they feel they understand is likely what
Hawkins was hinting at in his quote. The feeling of understanding is the
enemy of knowledge because it motivates you to stop acquiring the
stuff. It might even motivate you to begin to share that information
with others, opting to speak on a topic, rather than defer to who you
perceive to be an expert, but I won’t deal with that point here.
Given
that people often do not ever seem to reach complete understanding per
se, why should we ever expect people to stop trying to improve? Part of
that reason is that there’s a tradeoff between investing time in one
aspect of your life versus investing it in any other. Time spent
learning more about one skill is not time not spent doing other
potentially-useful things. Further still, were you to plot a learning
curve, charting how much new knowledge is gained per-unit of time
invested in learning, you’d likely see diminishing returns over time.
Let’s say you were trying to learn how to play a song on some musical
instrument. The first hour you spend practicing will result in you
gaining more information than, say, the thirtieth hour. At some point in
your practicing, you’ll reach a point where the value-added by each
additional hour simply isn’t worth the investment anymore. It is at this
point, when some cognitive balance
shifts away from investing time on learning one task to doing other
things, that we should predict people to reach a strong feeling of
understanding. Just as hunger wanes with each additional bite of food,
feelings of understanding should grow with each additional piece of
information.
Also like hunger,
some people tend a touch more towards the gluttonous side.
This brings us to the final question: what can we predict
about people’s behavior on the basis of their feelings of understanding?
Aside from the above mentioned disinclination to learn about some
specific topic further, we might also predict that repeated exposure to
information we feel we already understand would be downright aversive
(again, in much the same way that eating food after you feel full is
painful). We might, for instance, expect people to react with boredom
and diverted attention in classes that cover material too slowly. We
might also expect people to react with anger when someone
tries to explain something to them that they feel they already
understand. In fact, there is a word for what people consider that
latter act: condescending. Not only does condescension waste an
individual’s time with redundant information, it could also serve as an
implicit or explicit challenge to their social status via a challenge to
their understanding per se (i.e. “You think you understand this topic,
but you really don’t. Let me say it explain it to you
again…nice…and…slowly…). While this list is quite modest, I feel it
represents a good starting point for understanding understanding. Of
course, since I feel that way, there’s a good chance I’ll probably stop
looking for other starting points, so I may never know.
No comments:
Post a Comment